International Journal
on Marine Navigation
and Safety of Sea Transportation
Volume 3
Number 1
March 2009
67
Comparison of Traditional and Integrated
Bridge Design with SAGAT
F. Motz, E. Dalinger, H. Widdel & S. Hockel
Research Institute for Communication, Information Processing and Ergonomics,
Wachtberg, Germany
S. MacKinnon
Memorial University of Newfoundland, St. John’s, Canada
1 INTRODUCTION
Modern ship bridges are highly-automated man-
machine systems. Safety and efficiency of the ship
operations are dependent upon the ability of a
watchkeeper to perceive, interpret, and make deci-
sions upon information acquired from the surround-
ing environment. In the last years a strong increase
of modern information systems on ship bridges
could be observed. Simple displays and control sys-
tems were supplemented or replaced by complex
computer-based information systems. In order to
support the mariner effectively onboard, a task- and
situation-dependent representation of the informa-
tion is a compelling need. Modular Integrated Navi-
gation Systems (INS) according to the revised IMO
performance standards on INS (IMO 2007) combine
and integrate the validated information of different
sensors and functions and allow the presentation on
the various displays according to the tasks.
The aim of the investigations discussed within
this paper was to assess the advantages of an INS
design compared to a traditional bridge layout with
respect to the execution of collision avoidance and
route monitoring tasks. The Situation Awareness
Global Assessment Technique (SAGAT) (Endsley
2000) approach was applied to assess Situation
Awareness (SA) during these operations while
bridge design and out of window visibility were ma-
nipulated. The method to assess the SA of watch-
keeping officers on ship bridges was developed
based on previous studies (Motz et al. 2008).
The experiment was carried out in the full mis-
sion bridge simulator of the Centre for Marine Simu-
lation (CMS) of Memorial University of New-
foundland, St. John’s, Canada. The experimental tri-
als were conducted in a full bridge environment and
carried out with four scenarios, to investigate the in-
fluence of bridge design and outside visibility on the
SA of watchkeeping officers. The subjects were
tasked to navigate a vessel in scenarios with varying
traffic situations. In the trials a watch hand over was
simulated so that after the first 10 minutes of moni-
toring and evaluating the traffic situation the subject
assumed full control of the vessel.
ABSTRACT: Modular Integrated Navigation Systems (INS) according to the revised IMO performance stan-
dards on INS combine and integrate the validated information of different sensors and functions and allow the
presentation on the various displays according to the tasks. The aim of the investigations discussed within this
paper was to assess the advantages of an INS design compared to a traditional bridge layout with respect to
the execution of collision avoidance and route monitoring tasks. The Situation Awareness Global Assessment
Technique (SAGAT) approach was applied to assess Situation Awareness (SA) during these operations while
bridge design and out of window visibility were manipulated. Additionally, workload was measured by
NASA-TLX with its six subscales. The experiment was conducted in a full immersive simulation environ-
ment with 26 experienced mariners. The results indicate that SA is significantly higher with the INS bridge
design in the reduced visibility condition compared to the traditional bridge design. Also, tendencies were
found that workload and subdimensions are influenced by bridge design and visibility conditions.
68
2 SIMULATOR AND BRIDGE DESIGN
The experiment was carried out in a 6 degree of
freedom motion base, full mission ships bridge sim-
ulator (see Fig. 1). The simulator was manufactured
by Kongsberg Maritime Ship System. All trials were
conducted under a repeated motion profile. In the
experiment two bridge configurations were com-
pared: a traditional layout employing the existing
navigational equipment and an INS design. The ma-
jor difference between the two designs is the factor
of integration of the collision avoidance and naviga-
tion information, including route monitoring and
planning. The arrangement, location and design of
the equipment of both configurations were identical.
Figure 1. Full mission ships bridge simulator.
The following set up was used as traditional con-
figuration:
radar with facilities to display tracked radar tar-
gets
electronic Charting Display and Information Sys-
tem (ECDIS) with route information
minimum keyboard Display (MKD) to display
AIS target information
depth sounding information
heading information
speed information
VHF communication (Navtex, VHF DSC)
propulsion status displays
steering and engine control
steering status displays
alarm information presented on the individual
equipment
chart table with paper charts.
For the INS configuration the information of the
various navigational systems were integrated and
combined in the displays on the bridge:
collision avoidance display: radar with tracked
radar targets and AIS targets, possibility to under-
lay ENC chart information
route monitoring display: ECDIS with active
route and AIS targets
conning display: position, propulsion informa-
tion, rate of turn, relative wind speed and direc-
tion, engine alarm status, rudder indicator, gyro
repeater and speed
minimum keyboard Display (MKD) to display
AIS target information
speed information
VHF communication (Navtex, VHF DSC)
propulsion status displays
steering and engine control
steering status displays
alarm information presented on the individual
equipment
chart table with paper charts.
3 SAGAT
Situation Awareness is generally understood as
"knowing what is going around you". Within the re-
search community the definition of Endsley (1995)
has been widely accepted in various domains. In a
cognition-oriented approach, the model considers
three levels and includes:
perception of elements,
comprehension of the meaning of the elements
and the situation, and the
projection of the status of the elements and the
situation into the immediate future.
According to this model, decision making and
performance is influenced by SA.
The Situation Awareness Global Assessment
Technique (SAGAT) is probably one of the most
well known SA measuring techniques (Endsley
2000). It provides an objective explicit measure of
SA by directly comparing the operator’s SA to an
operational “scenario”. With this technique, a simu-
lation is frozen at a specific time, the system dis-
plays are blanked while the operator quickly has to
answer questions concerning the scenario. Tempo-
rary freezes in the simulation must be of a short du-
ration to minimise intrusiveness and memory decay.
To get an accurate measure of the operator’s SA the
SAGAT probes must cover all three levels of SA
and must be reflective of a wide range of the SA re-
quirements. These are delineated through a goal-
directed task analysis.
Table 1. Samples of SA questions.
Level Question
Perception
What is the current position of your ves-
sel?
What is the course of the blue highlighted
vessel?
Comprehension
What is the distance to the next waypoint?
What is the direction of the course change
of the highlighted vessel?
Projection
What is the CPA of the blue highlighted
vessel?
In how many minutes will you reach the
pilot station?
69
The method originally was developed for the avi-
ation domain (e.g. Endsley 1990, Strater & Endsley
2000) and has been applied in identical or modified
forms in other domains. Presently, it was adapted to
the marine domain (Motz et al. 2008). A sample of
SAGAT probes for marine application is shown in
Table 1.
SAGAT can be a useful tool to evaluate system
design. SAGAT can provide a form of diagnostic in-
formation that indicates how a technology’s design
could improve or weaken an operator’s SA when
compared to a baseline technology. This information
can then be used to refine design concepts. For the
ergonomic evaluation of the task- and situation ori-
entated presentation of navigational information on
INS it was considered to use the concept of situation
awareness measured with SAGAT.
Questions related to perception of elements (level
1) refer to the status of own ship as well as dynamics
of relevant objects in the environment. A mariner
has to possess correct information of own ship (posi-
tion, route, course etc.) as well as correct infor-
mation about targets (speed, distance etc.).
Questions related to comprehension of meaning
(level 2) go beyond simply being aware of the ele-
ments that are presented. An understanding of the
significance of those elements in light of mariner’s
goals is included. For example, a mariner must
quickly determine those targets which pose a threat
and eventually demand operator action to mediate a
threat or obstacle.
Questions related to projection of the near future
(level 3) refer to future actions of the elements in the
environment. This is achieved through knowledge of
the status and dynamic of the elements and a com-
prehension of the situation.
4 EXPERIMENT
4.1 Subjects
26 experienced mariners (masters, navigational offi-
cers, pilots) participated in the trials. The require-
ments for subject recruitment were:
at least half a year experience as officer of the
watch (OOW)
the mariners must have navigated a vessel in the
last four years
or working actively as navigational simulator in-
structors.
4.2 Hypotheses
It was hypothesized that navigating with INS bridge
design leads to higher SA scores than navigating
with traditional bridge design. This effect might be
more distinctive under difficult conditions like re-
duced outside view when fog prevails than under
good view. In the case of bad navigation conditions
also mental workload may be increased.
4.3 Experimental Design
A 2 x 2 factorial design with two within-subject fac-
tors was used. The first within-subject factor bridge
design varied on the two levels:
traditional configuration
INS configuration.
The second within-subject factor visibility varied
on the two levels:
good visibility
reduced visibility (fog).
Dependent variables were SA and NASA-TLX
scores (Hart & Staveland 1988).
A 2 x 2 repeated measures ANOVA (Analysis of
Variance) model with the within subject factors
bridge design and visibility were employed to ana-
lyse these data. As dependent variables the relative
frequency of correct answers representing the SA
score were analyzed. For workload the NASA-TLX
scores were used. All statistical interpretations were
considered at the 5 % level of significance.
4.4 Experimental Control
The experiment was carried out in the ships bridge
simulator with the two bridge designs described in
Section 2.
To control the presentation of SA questions on
the bridge, to log the answers of the subjects and to
monitor the vessels involved in a scenario an exper-
imental controlling program was developed by Re-
search Institute for Communication, Information
Processing and Ergonomics. The program consists
of the question presentation module, the nautical
chart module presenting the graphical information
for the SA questions and the experimental control
module (see Fig. 2). The three different modules
were located on different computers in the network.
Whereas the nautical chart module and the question
presentation module were installed on the naviga-
tional bridge of the simulator, the experimental con-
trol module runs on a PC in the simulator control
room.
Figure 2. Controlling components of experiment.
70
The experimental control module fulfilled the
three main tasks processing of simulator output, tar-
get control and control of SA questioning.
The experimental control module read and pro-
cessed the data of own ship’s GPS output signal and
the AIS signals of the other vessels provided to the
navigational equipment (e.g., the radar, ECDIS or
the MKD) on the bridge. This allowed the SA ques-
tions to be posed using real-time data. Therefore, the
software offered the possibility to show during the
simulation the movement of own ships and the other
vessels on a chart background and to offer sugges-
tions to change the course or speed of the vessels in
case of course or speed alterations of the own ship in
order to present the SA questions in similar traffic
situations for all participants.
4.5 Presentation of SA questions on the bridge
When the scenarios were “frozen” to present the
subjects the SA questions, the simulation was sus-
pended, all task relevant information was removed
from the navigational equipment on the bridge and
the outside view was blanked. The SA questions
were then administered on two displays on the
bridge. Whereas on one of the displays the questions
were presented, on the other display additional
graphical information for certain questions were dis-
played (see Fig. 2).
This offered the four techniques to ask the SA
questions, depending on the topic, complexity and
the easiest way to present and to answer the ques-
tions:
numeric open-end questions: Questions appeared
on the question display as numeric open-end
questions, e.g., what is your heading after the
next waypoint. No graphical information was pre-
sented on the graphical information display. To
answer the questions the subjects had to type in
the right answer and then to click on the continue
button to proceed with the next question
open-end questions with graphical answer: The
question after the position of the vessel appeared
on the question display as open-end question (in-
struction) and had to be answered on the graph-
ical information display by clicking on the chart
background.
numeric open-end questions with additional
graphical information: Questions appeared on the
question display as open-end questions with addi-
tional information presented on the chart of the
graphical information display, e.g., the target in-
volved in the question. To answer the questions
the subjects had to type in the right answer and
then click on the continue button to proceed with
the next question
multiple choice questions with additional graph-
ical information: Questions appeared on the ques-
tion display as multiple choice questions and on
the graphical information display question related
information was presented on the chart back-
ground. To answer the questions the subjects had
to select the right answer and then to click on the
continue button to proceed with the next question.
A preliminary question-pool of about 70 ques-
tions had been developed referring to the navigation
of the vessel focusing on route monitoring and colli-
sion avoidance. The questions were evaluated in pre-
tests which had the aim of selecting the most rele-
vant questions, of testing the content and under-
standability, and to reduce the number of questions
resulting in a set of 16 questions on three levels (see
Table 1 for examples).
4.6 Traffic scenarios
The SAGAT approach required the development of
realistic scenarios based on specific criteria, e.g.,
course change of own ship, navigational hazards,
traffic density, and “interest/danger” of targets. The
criteria for traffic density and “interest” of targets
are:
total number of targets - 20
number of targets within a 3 NM range - 10
number of targets within a 3 NM range: close
quarter targets (CPA: 0.5 - 1.5 NM); with a colli-
sion course, overtaking own ship or overtaken by
own ship - 5
number of targets, which cause a reaction because
of collision course - 1.
Figure 3: Singapore scenario at the first freezing.
On the basis of these criteria four traffic scenarios
with duration of 21.5 - 25 minutes were developed,
as well as one familiarization/habituation scenario.
To ensure that the previous knowledge of traffic and
of the sea area doesn’t influence the results (i.e. a
learning effect was controlled for), the scenarios rep-
resent different traffic situations for the Juan de Fuca
Strait / Strait of Georgia (familiarization scenario),
English Channel and Singapore. Figure 3 shows one
Singapore scenario at the time of the first freezing.
In the pre-trials the traffic scenarios were evaluated
in respect to realism, relevance and complexity.
71
4.7 Experimental procedure
The experimental procedure had five steps.
In the first step the experiment was introduced to
the subjects in a briefing outside the simulator. Sub-
jects had to complete a personal data sheet, which
was used to gather data like current occupation,
years of experience as mariner, and age, and the in-
tention of the experiment and general description of
experimental set up were described to the subjects.
In the second step subjects were familiarized in
the simulator with the experimental procedure and
the bridge equipment. The INS design and the tradi-
tional layout were explained in detail and the proce-
dure with the presentation of scenarios and the fol-
lowing interruptions for SA questioning were
explained.
In the third step familiarization trials were con-
ducted, one familiarization trial for each bridge lay-
out. The purpose was to familiarize the subject with
the navigation of the vessel with the different bridge
layouts, with the experimental procedure of the
freezings and with the different types of SA ques-
tions. The familiarization trials were carried out
without motion and with good visibility for all trials.
In the next step the four scenarios were presented
to the subject in a randomized order. The task of the
subject was to navigate a vessel in traffic situations
of varying density with either good or reduced visi-
bility. In the trials a watch hand over was simulated
so that the first 10 minutes of each scenario the sub-
ject monitored and evaluated the traffic situation. An
instructor was fulfilling the role of the officer of the
watch for the first 10 minutes. After the hand over,
the subject was in full control of the vessel. An in-
structor remained on the bridge and acted as both the
helmsman and the master. Thus, as the helmsman,
the instructor performed any changes in speed and
course and as master, to deny inappropriate deci-
sions of the subject that might disrupt the whole ex-
periment.
During each scenario there happened four freez-
ings in which the outside view and the displays were
blanked and the SA questions were asked. The first
freezing was conducted at the watch hand over and
the last at the end of the scenario. Same questions
were asked for all treatments, 16 questions per sce-
nario divided into 4 groups of 4 questions.
At the end of each scenario the NASA-TLX rat-
ing scale was completed. Following the collection of
all four scenarios the NASA-TLX rating paired
comparisons questionnaire were completed.
The duration of a simulation trial (4 scenarios and
habituation) per subject was between 190 to 220
minutes, depending on the time each subject needed
to become familiarize with the bridge equipment.
The trials were carried out with motion.
After the trials a SAGAT debriefing question-
naire and a INS questionnaire to evaluate certain as-
pects of an INS layout were administered to the sub-
jects in a separate room.
5 RESULTS
5.1 Main results
The central questions of the experiment were fo-
cused on the impact of the independent variables,
bridge design and visibility, on situation awareness.
The means of frequencies of correctly answered SA
questions are shown in Figure 4. The results of the
ANOVA for the within subject factors bridge design
and visibility show a significant main effect for the
factor bridge design (F
1,25
= 4.88, p < 0.05) and a
significant interaction effect between bridge design
and visibility (F
1,25
= 6.94, p < 0.05). No significant
effect for the factor visibility (F
1,25
= 0.94, p > 0.3)
could be found.
Means of correct answers [%]
trad. bridge
INS
Figure 4. Dependency of SA from bridge design and visibility.
The analysis of variance executed for the overall
workload, defined by the total score of NASA-TLX,
does not result in significant differences for the two
main effects visibility (F
1,25
= 3.57, p = 0.07) and
bridge design (F
1,25
=1.01, p=0.32) or for the interac-
tion (F
1,25
= 0.13, p = 0.72). But a strong tendency
can be seen that the INS produces less workload
than a traditionally designed bridge, and reduced
visibility is responsible for higher workload. Alt-
hough, tendencies were found for the subscales per-
formance and effort favoring the INS bridge design
especially under the condition of reduced visibility.
Results from the INS questionnaire support the
empirical data collected in this study. In general, the
majority of the subjects (93%) preferred the INS
bridge design compared to traditional design. The
participants who preferred the traditional bridge
gave as reasons that they are more used to the tradi-
tional design and that the INS design provides too
much information. As added value of an INS almost
72
all participants chose the answer “the combination of
information” (see Fig. 5). Half of the respondents se-
lected “the integrity of data” (meaning the possibil-
ity to compare automatically data from independent
sources). Less often selected is “the higher quality of
information”.
93
41
52
15
0
50
100
Combination Higher quality Integrity Other
Frequency of answers [%]
Figure 5. Means of frequency for the added value of an INS
bridge.
5.2 Post-hoc analysis
The definition of SA specifies a hierarchical struc-
ture with three levels (see Section 3). Questions for
the first level are preconditions for answering ques-
tions on level 2 and 3. Following this hierarchical
organization leads to the assumption that questions
of level 1 are answered correctly more often than
questions of the higher levels.
In the post-hoc analysis the factor level of SA was
included. A 2 x 2 x 3 ANOVA with the factors
bridge design, visibility and level of SA as within
subject factors was performed to test the assumption
which was justified by a significant main effect of
the factor level of SA (F
2,50
= 43.47, p < 0.001).
A pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correc-
tion of the three SA level scores show a significant
higher score for level 1 compared to level 2 and lev-
el 3, but no difference between the latter two. In
Figure 6 the means of frequencies of correct answers
for the 3 levels of SA are shown for condition re-
duced visibility.
35
45
55
65
75
SA Level 1 SA Level 2 SA Level 3
Means of correct answers [%]
trad. bridge
INS
Figure 6. Dependency of SA from bridge design on the three
SA levels.
SA level 1 (perception) had a greater score than
SA levels 2 and 3 (comprehension and projection),
suggesting that the application of SAGAT in this
maritime-related research was a valid approach to
assess global SA.
6 CONCLUSIONS
The results indicate that bridge design has a signifi-
cant impact on the degree of situation awareness, as
hypothesized. SA is significantly higher with the
INS bridge design in the reduced visibility condition
compared to the traditional bridge design. In good
visibility the SA is similar with both bridge designs.
Mariners have to rely more on information systems
when navigating in reduced visibility conditions. It
can be hypothesized that not only reduced visibility
but detrimental navigational conditions, in general,
may reduce SA when navigating with traditionally
designed bridges but not with INS. These considera-
tions also apply to workload in the sense that stress
inducing work conditions can influence total work-
load and subdimensions of NASA-TLX like perfor-
mance and effort when using traditional bridge de-
sign.
Further experiments are required comprising
more difficult navigation surroundings, e.g., higher
traffic density, more challenging navigation tasks,
high stress inducing work environment, to confirm
and sharpen these experimental findings.
REFERENCES
Endsley, M.R. 1990. A methodology for the objective meas-
urement of situation awareness. In Situational Awareness in
Aerospace Operations (AGARD-CP-478): 1-9. Neuilly sur
Seine, France: NATO- AGARD.
Endsley, M.R. 1995. Toward a theory of situation awareness in
dynamic systems. Human Factors 37: 32-64.
Endsley, M.R. 2000. Direct Measurement of Situation Aware-
ness. In M.R. Endsley & D.J. Garland (eds.), Situation
Awareness Analysis and Measurement: 147-173. Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hart, S.G. & Staveland, L.E. 1988. Development of NASA-
TLX (Task Load Index): Results of empirical and theoreti-
cal research. In P. Hancock & N. Meshkati (eds.), Human
Mental Workload: 139-183. Amsterdam: North Holland.
IMO 2007. Revised performance standards for integrated navi-
gation systems (INS). MSC.252(83). London: International
Maritime Organization.
Motz, F., MacKinnon, S., Dalinger, E., Widdel, H. 2008. Ap-
plication of SAGAT for Navigational Tasks on Ship Bridg-
es. International Maritime Conference / ABCD Symposium
on Human Performance at Sea. Sydney, Australia.
Strater, L. & Endsley, M.R. 2000. SAGAT- A situation aware-
ness measurement tool for commercial airline pilots. In
D.B. Kaber & M.R. Endsley (eds.), Proceedings of the first
human performance, situation awareness, and automation:
User-centered design for the new millennium conference:
360. Savannah, GA.